Doctor of Philology Mykhaylenko Valery V.

Chernivtsi University, Ukraine

Tag As Afterthought In Discourse

          The discourse analysis reveals reveals the elements structuring the discourse itself Their structural, semantic, functional, and pragmatic features have been under analysis  for a long time [4]. However, their attidudinal aspect has not been focused yet.

          There is a  tendency to add elements as  the Speaker’s  afterthought to the utterance expressed or the discourse support stimulating the Recepient to agree, to disagree, to confirm, to reject, or to comment the Speaker’s idea.    

          The grammarian sees discourse as having a definite structure which is the projection of rules which the language user is constrained to apply as a condition on his/her communicating at all. S/he does not see it, as the ethnomethodologists appear to see it, as a process whereby the language user resorts to certain strategies to apply his/her previous knowledge, or, to put it another way, where the language user has a knowledge of rules of application as well as a knowledge of rules relating to the language system. (Sacks 1974)

        Attempts to draw features of discourse within the scope of grammatical description, then, involve removing the distinction between a knowledge of the language system and the procedures which the language user follows in putting this knowledge to use in actual acts of communication. In consequence, discourse

is represented as consisting of a combination of pre-established meanings, as a set

of tokens of types already set up within the code of the language.

             Discourse consists of its discoursive units which can be called “quants”  creating a discourse, They function as nodes of graphs reflecting discoursive relationships between the parts of the discourse in terms of the Theory of Rhetorical Structures suggested by W.C.Mann and S.A.Thompson [3, 243-281].

            There are two criteria of defining a discourse constituent – semantic syntactic and intonational.  Accordingly, the first criterion considers predication to be an integral part, while the other requires an intonational pattern. The problem arises when there is a discrepancy between these two criteria.

            In semantics, discourses are linguistic units composed of several sentences — in other words, conversations, arguments or speeches. The study of discourses, or of language used by members of a speech community, is known as discourse analysis. It looks at both language form and function, and includes the study of both spoken interaction and written texts. It is a cross-disciplinary field, originally developing from sociolinguistics, anthropology, sociology and social psychology. dialogue between Speakers.

            Our hypothesis is that discourse markers can have one more function, that of a modus one. Modality in general – as one subcategory of the system of ‘tense aspect modality marking’, or of ‘qualifications of states of affairs’ remains among the major problem domains in linguistic semantics [5]  .

            But this characterization applies even more stringently to deontic modality in particular, as one modal subcategory, which in the literature is usually defined in terms of notions such as permission and obligation, and which is much less understood than its more ‘popular’ sister category of epistemic modality. Epistemic modality involves an assessment of the likelihood of a state of affairs, as expressed e.g. by the adverb probably in John is probably home now.

          There is practically no functional linguistic (including cognitive linguistic and typological) literature specifically devoted to the semantic notion of deontic modality, concern with it in linguistic semantics is nearly exclusively as a ‘byproduct’ in the context of analyses of the formal category of the modal auxiliaries (even more so than this is the case for epistemic modality, which has received at least some attention beyond the modals).

         Not surprisingly, then, there is no stable, well-founded and broadly accepted view regarding its status among the modal or more generally the qualificational dimensions, and even the few ‘default’ assumptions about it in the literature turn out to be problematic if put in a wider perspective. 

              Thus, deontic modality is not even generally acknowledged as a separate or independent semantic notion: specifically, it is often not separated from (parts of) ‘dynamic modality’. The latter category most typically involves an indication of abilities/possibilities or needs/necessities of the agent participant in the state of affairs, inherent ones, as expressed by the modal auxiliary. Also the ‘average’ definition of the category of deontic modality found in the literature appears problematic, however.

                  The object of our investigation is a tag as the Speaker’s afterthought element in discourse, e.g.:

                 You get right to the heart of the matter, don’t you? (D. Brin)

                 It is a pity we must go back to Shadizar empty handed, is it not? (R.Jordan)

Which can be analysed in the domain including also:

                 To what conclusion?  (D. Brin)

                Well then? (D. Brin)

                Quite a sight, eh? (D. Brin)

               Glory? (D. Brin)

              And why not? (D. Brin)

              The what? (D. Brin)

             Who better to choose as our guest’s bodyguard, eh? (D. Brin)

            You won’t tell, will you? (D. Brin)

 Studying segmentation markers should help determine how Speakers and Writers signal the beginning and end of large and small discourse units and how Listeners and Readers recognize these units. As soon as the language sciences expanded their research area beyond the sentence, researchers tried to demonstrate the existence of these units larger than the sentence and shorter than the text; they started looking for linguistic and paralinguistic marks that highlight the boundaries of these units.

                 The interest for a cognitive model of language understanding directed the attention to devices that would facilitate the integration of large amount of information. Markers highlighting the organization of discourse in chunks are well designed to fulfill this function. Finally, as the duty of introducing these markers in the discourse rests on the author/speaker, the way they are used should inform about the mechanisms of discourse generation, the contextual factors that control it, and the resources devoted to it by the Speaker and Writer. Segmentation markers can be seen as traces of the difficulties the Speaker encounters when they have to introduce of topic change in their discourse. The content is not recovered in the form of a coherent and cohesive text, but as chunks of closely related events. The chunk boundaries are a locus of significant change in time, space and casting. Updating all these constituents and retrieving the next chunk is costly in terms of cognitive effort and causes the occurrence of traces of segmentation. In written production, one should also expect to observe some traces of text segmentation provided that they are not removed by the author's editing work. Interjections of"er... hm" type  are not always the trace of difficulties in generating a topic hift, but can be intentionally introduced to inform the addressee about the presence   of a topic shift. Understanding a text is an incremental process in which new sentences are integrated with the preceding ones to construct a coherent mental representation. According to the Given-New Contract Theory, authors are expected to produce their discourse in such a way that Readers can apply the nextness principle. However, each time a new topic is introduced in the discourse they have to explicitly prevent the application of this principle.  According to the Gricean maxims of communication, Speakers and Writers are expected to inform the Addressees that continuity is not preserved, that there is a topic shift, and that special action should be taken. This is definitely an important pragmatic function of the segmentation marker. Yet, it is very difficult to find segmentation markers that would function exclusively as a trace or as a signal.

            In the existing literature about discourse markers it is possible to identify two approaches. On the one hand there are investigations with a pragmatic and textual orientation, which try to define theoretically the discourse mechanisms governing discourse markers use. On the other hand there are more empirical studies, often with computational purposes, which concentrate on the lexical aspects of discourse markers. It is well known that within the pragmatic/textual orientation there are two distinct approaches: the Discourse-Coherence approach (Schiffrin[8]   Redeker [6], Fraser[2]) and the Relevance-Theory approach (Blackemore[1]    ) Discourse markers bear some symbolic information and play the function of linking the context with the utterance making the information accessable to the Reader/Listener.  The functions of discourse markers are in  various contexts: converasation managing, role change, topic shifts, separation, utterance coherence, developing a macrostructure, pause fillers, discourse relationship, hesitating markers, modus markers (speaker’s subjective modality markers),expressions of attitudes, hedgings markers, supporting markers, etc.

           We use the term discourse marker for those lexical items that (in addition to non-lexical means such as punctuation, aspectual and focus shifts, etc.) can signal the presence of such a relation at the linguistic surface. Definitions of discourse markers have often been couched in negative terms: markers are said to be non-propositional, to contribute nothing to truth-conditional meaning, to have little or no semantic value, to be outside the syntax of the sentence, to be optional elements, etc. The discoursal use of such expressions is part of the wider phenomenon of Speakers' attitudes towards the ideas they express. The study of discourse markers is therefore a part of the study of modal and metatextual comment, and is best approached under the rubric of discourse structure.

             Discourse markers are a polifunctional and polisemantic    phenomenon, therefore they can be investigated in various formats – the thery of speech acts, the theory of speech relativity, discourse analysis, and others. For a quarter of a century some six fundamental approaches have been employed: the  theory of discourse (Schiffrin),  the  theory of linguistics (Fraser),  the  theory of coherence (Sanders), the theory of relevence òåîðèÿ (Blakemore), the theory of argumentation  (Ducrot and Anscombre), the cognitive theory (Sweetser).

      The difference lies in understanding the functions of discourse markers and methods of their analysis. Consequently we have functional pragmatic linguistics of discourse markers, sociolinguistics of discourse markers, and diachrony of discourse markers. The classification of discourse markers is based on the following features: contents, propositional structure, speech act structure, interchange structure,  participants structure, information structure, form, function, distribution.

                  Discourse structure is a crucial component in both language interpretation and generation. In interpretation, discourse structure provides clues about how people infer content that's linguistically implicit. In generation, discourse structure is needed for planning text which effectively communicates the intended content, and which sounds natural in that it doesn't make every bit of that content linguistically explicit. Most tags are loosely attached to the preceding sentence. There are various kinds of tag are registered: tag-questions, retrospective comment clauses, retrospective vagueness hedges, noun phrase tags, other non-clausal units retrospectively added, self-supplied answers,vocatives.

Bidliography:

1.Blakemore D. Relevance and Linguistic Meaning: The Semantics and Pragmatics of Discourse Markers. – Cambridge:  CUP, 2003.

2.Fraser B. 1996. Pragmatic Markers. // Pragmatics.-1996. - 6(2).–1996. – Pp.167-90.

3.Mann W.C., Thompson S.A. Rhetorical structure theory: toward a functional theory of text organization. // Text. – 1988. – 8. – Pp. 243-281.

4. Mykhaylenko V. Tag-Questions in Discourse //Ìàòåð³ëè ²² ̳æíàðîäíî¿ íàóêîâî-ïðàêòè÷íî¿ êîíôåðåíö³¿ «Äí³ íàóêè –«2006». Òîì 18. Ô³ëîëîã³÷í³ íàóêè. – Äí³ïðîïåòðîâñüê: Íàóêà ³ îñâ³òà, 2006. – Ñ.53-57.

5.Nyan T. Metalinguistic Operators (With Reference to French).Manuscript. Dept. of French  Studies. - Manchester : University of  Manchester, 1995.

6.Redeker G. Ideational and Pragmatic Markers of Discourse Structure.// Journal of Pragmatics .- 1990. -14. – Pp. 367-381.

7.Sacks H., Schegloff E.,  Jefferson G. A Simplest Systematics of the organization Turn-Taking conversation //Language. -1974. -N50. – Pp.696-735

8.Schiffrin D. Discourse Markers: Language, Meaning, and Context. //D. Schiffrin, 9.Tannen D., Hamilton D. (Eds.). The Handbook of Discourse Analysis. - Oxford: Basil Blackwell, 2001. – Pp. 54-75.